Details
Title
Personality and Cognitive PerformanceJournal title
Polish Psychological BulletinYearbook
2008Issue
No 4Authors
Divisions of PAS
Nauki Humanistyczne i SpołecznePublisher
Committee for Psychological Science PASDate
2008Identifier
DOI: 10.2478/v10059-008-0025-4Source
Polish Psychological Bulletin; 2008; No 4References
Asendorpf J. (1983), The discrepant repressor: Differentiation between low anxiety, high anxiety, and repression of anxiety by autonomic-facial-verbal patterns of behaviour, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 1334, doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.45.6.1334 ; Badley A. (2001), Is working memory still working?, American Psychologist, 56, 851, doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.56.11.851 ; Baddeley A. (1986), Working memory. ; Borkovec T. (1994), Worrying: Perspectives on theory, assessment and treatment, 5. ; Bower G. (1981), Mood and memory, American Psychologist, 36, 129, doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.36.2.129 ; Bradley B. (1999), Attentional bias for emotional faces in generalised anxiety disorder, British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 38, 267, doi.org/10.1348/014466599162845 ; Byrne A. (1995), Trait anxiety, anxious mood, and threat detection, Cognition and Emotion, 9, 549, doi.org/10.1080/02699939508408982 ; Bullen J. (1984), Psychoticism and visual recognition threshold, Personality and Individual Differences, 5, 633, doi.org/10.1016/0191-8869(84)90121-1 ; Clore G. (2001), Handbook of Affect and Social Cognition. ; Coifman K. (2007), Does repressive coping promote resilience? Affective-autonomic responses discrepancy during bereavement, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 745, doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.92.4.745 ; Collette F. (2002), Brain imaging of the central executive component of working memory, Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 26, 105, doi.org/10.1016/S0149-7634(01)00063-X ; Corbetta M. (2002), Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in he brain, Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3, 201. ; Crowne D. (1960), A new scale of social desirability independent of psychopathology, Journal of Consulting Psychology, 24, 349, doi.org/10.1037/h0047358 ; Derakshan N. (2007), Emotional information processing in repressors: the vigilance-avoidance theory, Cognition and Emotion. ; Derryberry D. (2002), Anxiety-related attentional biases and their regulation by attentional control, Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111, 2, 225, doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.111.2.225 ; Diener E. (2003), Personality, culture, and subjective well-being: Emotional and cognitive evaluations of life, Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 403, doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145056 ; Drwal R. (1980), Opracowanie Kwestionariusza Aprobaty Społecznej, Przegląd Psychologiczny, 23, 3, 569. ; Dowens M. (2003), Genuine memory bias versus response bias in anxiety, Cognition and Emotion, 17, 6, 843, doi.org/10.1080/02699930244000381 ; Eliasz A. (1981), Temperament a system regulacji stymulacji. ; Eliasz A. (1985), Temperamental bases of behavior: Studies on individual differences, 41. ; Eliasz A. (1992), Temperament a rozwój młodzieży, 11. ; Eysenck H. (1967), The biological basis of personality. ; Eysenck H. (1981), A model of personality, 1. ; Eysenck H. (1992), The definition and measurement of psychoticism, Personality and Individual Differences, 13, 757, doi.org/10.1016/0191-8869(92)90050-Y ; Eysenck H. (1975), Manual of Eysenck Personality Questionnaire (Jounior and Adult). ; Eysenck H. (1985), Personality and individual differences. ; Eysenck H. (1994), Manual of the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire: Comprising the EPQ-Revised (EPQ-R) and EPQ-R Short Scale. ; Eysenck M. (2006), Podpatrywanie uczuć i myśli. Zaburzenia i optymalizacja procesów emocjonalnych i poznawczych. Nowe kierunki badań, 87. ; Eysenck M. (2000), A cognitive approach to trait anxiety, European Journal of Personality, 14, 463, doi.org/10.1002/1099-0984(200009/10)14:5<463::AID-PER393>3.0.CO;2-3 ; Eysenck M. (1997), Anxiety and cognition: a unified theory. ; Eysenck M. (1992), Anxiety: The cognitive perspective. ; Eysenck M. (1982), Attention and arousal: Cognition and performance. ; Eysenck M. (1979), Anxiety, learning, and memory: A reconceptualization, Journal of Research in Personality, 13, 363, doi.org/10.1016/0092-6566(79)90001-1 ; Eysenck M. (2007), Anxiety and cognitive performance: Attentional control theory, Emotion, 7, 336, doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.7.2.336 ; Eysenck M. (1992), Anxiety and performance: The processing efficiency theory, Cognition and Emotion, 6, 409, doi.org/10.1080/02699939208409696 ; Eysenck M. (1987), Cognitive functioning an anxiety, Psychological Research, 49, 189, doi.org/10.1007/BF00308686 ; Fajkowska M. (2007), Właściwości indywidualne i efekt twarzy w tłumie, Przegląd Psychologiczny. ; Fajkowska M. (2006), Podpatrywanie uczuć i myśli. Zaburzenia i optymalizacja procesów poznawczych i emocjonalnych, 45. ; Fajkowska M. (2006), Podpatrywanie uczuć i myśli. Zaburzenia i optymalizacja procesów poznawczych i emocjonalnych, 63. ; Fajkowska-Stanik M. (2003), Pułapki metodologiczne w badaniach empirycznych z zakresu psychologii klinicznej, 249. ; Fajkowska-Stanik M. (2003), Depresja i emocje: ujęcie transakcyjne, Studia Psychologiczne, 41, 1, 107. ; Fajkowska-Stanik M. (2005), Badanie trafności doboru schematycznych bodźców twarzy, Studia Psychologiczne, 43, 4, 91. ; Folk Ch. (1992), Involuntary covert orienting is contingent on attentional control settings, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 4, 1030, doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.18.4.1030 ; Fox E. (2008), Emotion Science. Cognitive and neuroscientific approaches to understanding human emotions. ; Fox E. (1993), Allocation of visual attention and anxiety, Cognition and Emotion, 7, 207, doi.org/10.1080/02699939308409185 ; Fox E. (2005), Anxiety modulates the degree of attentive resources required to process emotional faces, Cognitive, Affective, & BehavioralNeuroscience, 5, 396, doi.org/10.3758/CABN.5.4.396 ; Fox E. (2001), Do threatening stimuli draw or hold visual attention in subclinical anxiety?, Journal of Experimental Psychology, 130, 4, 681, doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.130.4.681 ; Fox E. (2002), Attentional bias for threat: Evidence for delayed disengagement from emotional faces, Cognition and Emotion, 16, 355, doi.org/10.1080/02699930143000527 ; Hebb D. (1955), Drives and the C. N. S. (conceptual nervous system), Psychological Review, 62, 243, doi.org/10.1037/h0041823 ; Hadwin J. (2005), State anxiety and working memory in children: A test of processing efficiency theory, Educational Psychology, 25, 379, doi.org/10.1080/01443410500041607 ; Hansen C. (1988), Finding the face in the crowd: An anger superiority effect, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 917, doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.917 ; Hoge E. (2007), Resilience: research evidence and conceptual considerations for posttraumatic stress disorder, Depression and Anxiety, 24, 139, doi.org/10.1002/da.20175 ; Humphreys M. (1984), Personality, motivation, and performance: A theory of the relationship between individual differences and information processing, Psychological Review, 91, 153, doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.91.2.153 ; Juth P. (2005), Looking for foes and friends: Perceptual and emotional factors when finding a face in the crowd, Emotion, 5, 379, doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.5.4.379 ; Lundqvist D. (1999), Face of the wrath: Critical features for conveying facial threat, Cognition and Emotion, 13, 691, doi.org/10.1080/026999399379041 ; Mackworth N. (1948), The brakedown of vigilance during prolonged visual search, Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1, 6, doi.org/10.1080/17470214808416738 ; Mackworth N. (1957), Some factors affecting vigilance, Advancements in Science, 53, 389. ; MacLeod C. (1988), Anxiety and the allocation of attention to threat, Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38A, 659. ; Matthews G. (1991), Self-report arousal and divided attention: a study of performance operating characteristics, Human Performance, 4, 107, doi.org/10.1207/s15327043hup0402_2 ; Mogg K. (2005), Attentional bias in generalized anxiety disorder versus depressive disorder, Cognitive Therapy and Research, 29, 1, 29, doi.org/10.1007/s10608-005-1646-y ; Mogg K. (2000), Trait anxiety, defensiveness and selective processing of threat: An investigation using two measures of attentional bias, Personality and Individual Differences, 28, 1063, doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(99)00157-9 ; Mogg K. (2004), Selective attention to angry faces in a clinical sample with social phobia, Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 113, 160, doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.113.1.160 ; Myers L. (2004), To forget or not forget:what do repressors forget and when do they forget?, Cognition and Emotion, 18, 495, doi.org/10.1080/02699930341000419 ; Myers L. (1996), The color naming of socially threatening words, Personality and Individual Differences, 20, 801, doi.org/10.1016/0191-8869(96)00019-0 ; Newman L. (2002), Repressive coping and threatavoidance: An idiographic Stroop study, Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 409, doi.org/10.1177/0146167202286011 ; Nęcka E. (2000), Pobudzenie intelektu. Zarys formalnej teorii inteligencji. ; Öhman A. (2005a), Conditioned fear of a face: A prelude to ethnic enmity?, Science, 309, 711, doi.org/10.1126/science.1116710 ; Öhman A. (2005b), The role of the amygdala in human fear: Automatic detection of threat, Psychoneurendocrinology, 30, 952, doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2005.03.019 ; Öhman A. (1993), Handbook of Emotions, 511. ; Öhman A. (1984), Sociophysiology, 47, doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4612-5202-3_3 ; Öhman A. (2001a), Emotion drives attention: detecting the snake in the grass, Journal of Experimental Psychology, 130, 3, 466, doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.130.3.466 ; Öhman A. (2001b), The face in the crowd revisited: a threat advantage with schematic stimuli, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 3, 381, doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.80.3.381 ; Öhman A. (2000), Handbook of psychophisiology, 533. ; Pavłov I. (1938/1952), Dwadzieścia lat badań wyższej czynności nerwowej (zachowania się) zwierząt. ; Pishyar R. (2004), Attenional bias for words and faces in social anxiety, Anxiety, Stress and Copying, 17, 1, 23, doi.org/10.1080/10615800310001601458 ; Purcell D. (1996), It takes a confounded face to pop out of a crowd, Perception, 25, 1091, doi.org/10.1068/p251091 ; Robinson M. (2006), The automaticity of affective reactions: stimulus valence, arousal, and lateral spatial attention, Social Cognition, 24, 4, 469, doi.org/10.1521/soco.2006.24.4.469 ; Santos, R., Wall, M. B., & Eysenck, M. W. (2007). <i>Anxiety and processing efficiency: fMRI evidence.</i> Manuscript submitted for publication ; Spielberger C. (1988), State-Trait Anger Expression Inventory, Research Edition. Professional Manual. ; Strelau J. (2008), Temperament as a regulator of behavior. After fifty years of research. ; Szymura B. (2007), Temperament uwagi. ; Szymura, B. & Śmigasiewicz, K. (manuscript). <i>Psychoticism and switching of attention.</i> ; Szymura B. (1998), Visual selective attention and personality: An experimental verification of three models of extroversion, Personality and Individual Differences, 24, 713, doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(97)00198-0 ; Szymura B. (2003), What really bothers neurotics? In search for factors impairing attentional performance, Personality and Individual Differences, 34, 109, doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(02)00034-X ; J. Van Honk (2001), Attentionalbiases for angry faces: Relationships to trait anger and anxiety, Cognition and Emotion, 15, 279, doi.org/10.1080/0269993004200222 ; Weinberger D. (1979), Low-anxious, high-anxious, andrepressive coping styles: Psychometric patterns and behavioural and physiological responses to stress, Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 88, 369, doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.88.4.369 ; Williams J. (1997), Cognitive psychology and emotional disorders. ; Wilson E. (2003), Contrasting two accounts of anxiety-linked attentional bias: Selective attention to varying levels of stimulus threat intensity, Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 112, 212, doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.112.2.212 ; Wrześniewski K. (1996), Inwentarz Stanu i Cechy Lęku (ISCL). Polska adaptacja STAI. Podręcznik. ; Yiend J. (2001), Anxiety and attention to threatening pictures, Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54A, 665, doi.org/10.1080/02724980042000462Aims and scope
Polish Psychological Bulletin (founded in 1970) is an official journal of Polish Academy of Sciences, Committee for Psychological Science.The journal publish a variety of papers, including empirical reports of experiments, surveys and field studies, theoretical articles, controversies and analytic papers on important psychological topics. Relevance for an international readership is our prominent goal, Polish Psychological Bulletin does not publish clinical case studies, or technical articles. Submissions from all domains of psychology are encouraged, especially those that address new developments and pursue innovative approaches.Periodically, the journal will announce a call for papers for special issues. The journal will also entertain unsolicited proposals for special issues that fit the stated scope of the Polish Psychiological Bulletin (please contact the journal’s Editor-in-Chief with a detailed description of your proposal).
All published research articles in this journal have undergone rigorous review, based on initial editor screening and anonymous evaluation of content and merit by independent expert reviewers.
For information on specific requirements, please see the Author Guidelines.
Abstracting & Indexing
Abstracting and Indexing Information
• DESY Publication Database
• Directory of Open Access Journal (DOAJ)
• Current Contents: Social & Behavioral Sciences
• Dimensions
• EBSCO
• ERIH Plus
• Google Scholar
• Index Copernicus
• ProQuest
• PsychArchives
• Science Open
• SCOPUS (Elsevier)
• Sherpa/RoMEO